Research Article |
Corresponding author: Tatsuru Kuga ( tkuga486@gmail.com ) Academic editor: Laurel B. Symes
© 2024 Tatsuru Kuga, Eiiti Kasuya.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Kuga T, Kasuya E (2024) Factors related to sound production by the Chinese grasshopper Acrida cinerea during escape. Journal of Orthoptera Research 33(1): 13-19. https://doi.org/10.3897/jor.33.100865
|
Many grasshopper species produce conspicuous sounds while escaping from approaching predators; however, they occasionally escape without producing sounds. The Chinese grasshopper, Acrida cinerea, often exhibits noisy escape behavior. Therefore, a field experiment was conducted using A. cinerea to identify factors related to the production of sound during escape. This study utilized a predator model with an investigator approaching A. cinerea three times. We examined the relationship between the production of sound during escape and the following factors: ambient temperature and relative humidity as environmental factors; sex, body length, body weight, and limb autotomy as prey traits; and the repeated approach as a predator trait. The relationships between noisy escape and flight initiation distance (i.e., predator-prey distance when the prey initiates the escape), distance fled (i.e., distance the prey covered during the escape), and the mode of locomotion during escape (i.e., flying or jumping) were also examined. Noisy escape was observed only in males that escaped by flying, whereas the females and males that escaped by jumping invariably escaped silently. Among males that flew, noisy escape was related to ambient temperature, limb autotomy, and distance fled. The proportion that produced sound increased in parallel with the ambient temperature and distance fled. This proportion was lower among individuals that had autotomized one of their hind legs. These results indicate that noisy escape behavior is most frequent in healthy male A. cinerea under warm conditions.
antipredator tactics, crepitation, distance fled, flight initiation distance, fly, jump, predator-prey interaction
Many animals exhibit conspicuous behavior when they escape from approaching predators (
Many species of grasshoppers produce sounds when they escape by flying (
This study examined the environmental factors as well as prey and predator traits that may be related to the noisy escape of the Chinese grasshopper A. cinerea (Fig.
As a function of conspicuous escape, the sudden disappearance of conspicuous behavior during the escape may confuse predators about the location of the prey and deter predators from searching for it (
Another function of conspicuous escape is to send a signal to predators that the prey has a good ability for escape and deter them from approaching the prey (
Many grasshopper species, including A. cinerea, escape via two locomotion modes: flying and jumping (
Study animals and study sites.—Acrida cinerea is commonly found in Japan and characteristically produces sound during flight (
Experimental procedure.—The escape behavior of grasshoppers is often induced by the approach of an investigator (
At each site, the investigator recorded the behavior of an individual grasshopper during three consecutive escapes. The experiments took place from 10:00 to 15:00 each day. An interval between experiments in the same sites was 1h or longer to minimize potential effects of the previous experiment that could influence the results of the next experiment (e.g., disturbance of the grass). Experiments were not conducted during periods of rain.
The experimental procedure that was followed at each site included the three steps below: identification of an individual, three consecutive approaches to the target, and capture of the target. The investigator searched for an individual A. cinerea while walking at one step per second (walking speed, mean ± standard deviation [SD] = 36.6 ± 1.0 cm/s, n = 20). The walking speed was maintained using the metronome sound from an audio player (MD720J/A, Apple, California, USA; METRONOME STAR app v.2.0.0, 60 beats/min). The same area in the site was never searched more than once during the experimental procedure. Following the identification of an individual A. cinerea, the investigator approached the target at the same walking speed. The first encounter with a target often occurred while the grasshopper was escaping, and this escape was regarded as the first attempt. The first approach was terminated when the target initiated the escape. Then, markers (wire rings with a diameter of 5 cm) were quickly placed on the investigator’s position and the initial location of the target grasshopper at the start of the first escape. The second and third approaches were conducted in the same manner immediately after the markers were placed. Following the three consecutive approaches, the investigator captured the grasshopper and placed a marker on the position of the grasshopper at the end of the third escape.
When the investigator failed in either the approach step or the capture step, the step of identification was restarted at the same site. The three steps were repeated at that site until the investigator accomplished all three steps or searched the whole area of the site for the target grasshopper. All captured grasshoppers were maintained in a laboratory (temperature: 22–26°C; food: mostly Paspalum urvillei Steudel) until the end of the study.
Measurements.—The sound produced by A. cinerea is detectable by the human ear. The investigator recorded whether sounds were produced by A. cinerea during the escape attempts. This data recording was confirmed using videos captured by the non-high-speed digital camera. Video analyses were conducted using the BORIS v.4.1.11 software (
Locomotion modes during escape attempts were classified according to the video recorded by the high-speed digital camera. Wing flapping after takeoff indicated flying, while lack of wing flapping after takeoff denoted jumping. A preliminary experiment showed that target grasshoppers often escaped outside the camera frame. To confirm the locomotion modes of targets outside the camera frame, the investigator observed the locomotion modes visually while approaching the target in the field. When wing flapping of the target was not recorded in the video but was observed in the field, the locomotion mode of that target was classified as flying.
FID and DF were recorded by measuring the distances between markers using a steel tape measure to the nearest 1 cm. FID was measured as the distance between two markers placed on the positions of the grasshopper and the investigator at the initiation of each escape attempt. DF was measured as the distance between the two markers placed at the positions of the grasshopper at the initiation and end of each escape attempt. The second and third escape attempts were induced immediately after the previous escape. Hence, the markers placed at the positions of the grasshopper at the initiation of the second and third escape attempts were considered to be placed at the positions of the grasshopper at the end of the first and second escape attempts, respectively. These measurements were conducted after capturing the target grasshopper.
The ambient temperature (to the nearest 0.01°C) and relative humidity (to the nearest 0.01%) were recorded after the capture of the target. We used a temperature and humidity data logger (Satoshoji LITE5032P-RH, Kanagawa, Japan) for the recording. During each experiment, the data logger was hung on a tree branch at a height of 140–200 cm.
The morphological traits of the individuals were measured in a single day after the end of the final experiment. Body weight to the nearest 0.01 g was measured using an electronic balance device (Sartorius 1416MP8, Göttingen, Germany). The grasshoppers defecated frass, thereby reducing their body weight between the time of collection and that of the measurement. Measurement of body weight at the time of escape was important to examine the relationship with noisy escape. Thus, the total weight of the grasshopper and its frass, rather than the grasshopper’s weight alone, was measured. Using a digital caliper, body length was measured to the nearest 0.01 mm as the distance from the tip of the head to the end of the forewings (Mitutoyo CD-20C, Kanagawa, Japan).
Statistical analyses.—The following statistical tests were conducted with R v.4.1.1 (
We examined the relationship between noisy escape, sex, and locomotion modes. The frequency of noisy escape was compared between males and females in each of the three consecutive escape attempts using Fisher’s exact test. This test was also used to examine sex differences in the frequency of each locomotion mode.
Factors related to sound production in the first escape attempt were examined using generalized linear models (GLMs) with a quasi-binomial error structure and logit link. The models were fitted to the data of males that flew in the first escape attempt because females and males that escaped by jumping did not produce sounds (see Results). The objective variable was sound production (no = 0; yes = 1) in the first escape attempt. The explanatory variables were ambient temperature, humidity, body length, body weight, limb autotomy (no = 0; yes = 1), FID, and DF in the first escape attempt. We also fitted models that contained a quadratic term of temperature or humidity as another explanatory variable to the data and examined the possibility that these parameters affect sound production quadratically. There were no significant effects found in these quadratic terms of temperature (coefficient ± standard error [SE] = –0.06 ± 0.03, t = –1.795, degree of freedom [df] = 125, p = 0.075) and humidity (coefficient ± SE = –0.002 ± 0.006, t = –0.323, df = 125, p = 0.747). Similarly, there were no significant effects of the quadratic terms of temperature and humidity when the model contained both these terms at the same time (temperature: t = –1.755, df = 124, p = 0.082; humidity: t = –0.182, df = 124, p = 0.856). Thus, these quadratic terms were removed from the model.
Changes in the frequency of noisy escape through repeated escape attempts were tested using the exact McNemar test. Changes in frequency were examined for each of the first and second escape attempts and for the second and third escape attempts. We used only the data of males that escaped by flying in the three escape attempts for this and the subsequent statistical tests on repeated escapes.
Factors that affect sound production during repeated escape attempts were examined using GLMs with quasi-binomial error structure and logit link. The objective variable was sound production in the second or third escape attempt. In the model for sound production during the second escape attempt, the explanatory variables were FID, DF, and sound production in the first escape attempt, as well as FID and DF in the second escape attempt. In the model for sound production during the third escape attempt, the explanatory variables were FID, DF, and sound production in the second escape attempt, as well as FID and DF in the third escape attempt.
We collected data on three consecutive escape attempts of 136 males and 13 females (Table
Numbers of individuals in the locomotion modes and sound production in three escape attempts.
Sex | Attempt | Locomotion mode | Sound production | |
---|---|---|---|---|
No | Yes | |||
Male | First | Fly | 32 | 102 |
Jump | 2 | 0 | ||
Second | Fly | 27 | 103 | |
Jump | 6 | 0 | ||
Third | Fly | 35 | 96 | |
Jump | 5 | 0 | ||
Female | First | Fly | 6 | 0 |
Jump | 7 | 0 | ||
Second | Fly | 5 | 0 | |
Jump | 8 | 0 | ||
Third | Fly | 3 | 0 | |
Jump | 10 | 0 |
Female A. cinerea did not produce conspicuous sounds regardless of the locomotion mode (Table
For males that flew in the first escape attempt, temperature, limb autotomy, and DF were significantly related to sound production (Table
Relationships between sound production and DF or FID in the first (A, B), second (C, D), and third (E, F) escape attempts. Data on the first attempt (A, B) were obtained from males that flew in the first attempt. Data for the second (C, D) and third (E, F) attempts were obtained from males that flew in all three consecutive escape attempts. The number of observed individuals is shown in parentheses. The centerline, lower edge, and upper edge of the box indicate the median, first quantile, and third quantile, respectively. The bottom of the lower whisker is the minimum value that is not lower than the first quantile minus 1.5 times the interquartile range. The top of the upper whisker is the maximum value that is not higher than the third quantile plus 1.5 times the interquartile range. The data points that are not contained in the box-and-whisker plot are represented as open circles.
Result of GLM on sound production (no = 0; yes = 1) in the first escape attempt. The error structure was quasi-likelihood (“quasibinomial” in GLM function of R), and the link function was logit. The model contained all the explanatory variables at the same time. Only data of males that escaped by flying in the first escape attempt were included in the analysis. Definitions/Abbreviations: Autotomy, the occurrence of autotomy of the hind leg (no = 0; yes = 1); Coefficient, estimated value of the coefficient; p, p-value of the statistical test on the coefficient; SE, standard error of the estimate of the coefficient; t, value of t-statistics (df = 126).
Explanatory variable | Coefficient | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | −4.919 | 8.952 | −0.550 | 0.584 |
Temperature | 0.266 | 0.121 | 2.196 | 0.030 |
Humidity | −0.059 | 0.049 | −1.216 | 0.226 |
Body length | 0.130 | 0.177 | 0.735 | 0.464 |
Weight | −17.453 | 9.323 | −1.872 | 0.064 |
Autotomy | −1.865 | 0.818 | −2.281 | 0.024 |
FID | −0.006 | 0.008 | −0.757 | 0.450 |
DF | 0.012 | 0.004 | 3.527 | <0.001 |
Some of the males that escaped by flying in all three consecutive escape attempts showed both noisy and silent flight (Fig.
In the second escape attempt, sound production was significantly related to DF (Table
Result of GLM on sound production (no = 0; yes = 1) in repeated escape attempts. The error structure was quasi-likelihood (“quasibinomial” in GLM function of R), and the link function was logit. The model contained all the explanatory variables at the same time. Only data of males that escaped by flying in all three consecutive escape attempts were included in the analyses. Definitions/Abbreviations: Coefficient, estimated values of the coefficient; DF1, DF2, and DF3, DF in the first, second, and third escape attempts, respectively; FID1, FID2, and FID3, FID in the first, second, and third escape attempts, respectively; SE, standard error of the estimate of the coefficient; Sound1, Sound2, and Sound3, sound production (no = 0; yes = 1) in the first, second, and third escape attempts, respectively; p, p-value of the statistical test on the coefficient; t: value of t-statistics (df = 120).
Objective variable | Explanatory variable | Coefficient | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sound2 | (Intercept) | −1.180 | 0.695 | −1.698 | 0.092 |
Sound1 | 1.014 | 0.584 | 1.737 | 0.085 | |
FID1 | 0.020 | 0.010 | 1.928 | 0.056 | |
FID2 | −0.014 | 0.010 | −1.332 | 0.185 | |
DF1 | −0.002 | 0.003 | −0.545 | 0.587 | |
DF2 | 0.009 | 0.004 | 2.602 | 0.010 | |
Sound3 | (Intercept) | −0.985 | 0.634 | −1.554 | 0.123 |
Sound2 | 2.081 | 0.552 | 3.774 | < 0.001 | |
FID2 | −0.005 | 0.008 | −0.601 | 0.549 | |
FID3 | −0.002 | 0.009 | −0.167 | 0.867 | |
DF2 | −0.002 | 0.003 | −0.527 | 0.599 | |
DF3 | 0.006 | 0.003 | 1.990 | 0.049 |
Sound production in the third escape attempt was significantly related to DF in the third escape attempt and sound production in the second escape attempt (Table
Observed number of male A. cinerea that escaped by flying in all three consecutive escape attempts. The horizontal axis shows the production of sound (N = No; Y = Yes) in three consecutive escape attempts in the order they took place. For example, YYN indicates that the grasshopper produced sounds in the first and second escape attempts but did not produce sound in the third attempt.
Sound was produced only by male A. cinerea that escaped by flying. Most of the males escaped by flying, whereas some escaped by jumping and did not produce sound. Even when the males escaped by flying, they did not always produce sound. Sound production by these males was related to ambient temperature, limb autotomy, and DF.
Ambient temperature was the only environmental factor found to be related to noisy escape. High ambient temperature results in a higher wingbeat frequency in many kinds of insects (
Limb autotomy was the only morphological trait of male A. cinerea found to be related to their noisy escape. Males that autotomized one of their hind legs produced sound less frequently than intact males. Many grasshoppers and locusts can autotomize their hind legs for survival (
Repeated approaches, as a predator trait, were not related to the frequency of noisy escape by male A. cinerea. The lizard Callisaurus draconoides Blainville, 1835, which escapes conspicuously by waving its tail, decreases the frequency of conspicuous escape through repeated escape attempts (
DF was related to the noisy escape of A. cinerea. Conspicuous escape was observed more frequently in males with longer DF. Three non-mutually exclusive hypotheses may explain this relationship. First, conspicuous behaviors by males with a longer DF might act as an honest signal of good ability for escape to deter predatory attacks, as suggested for the lizard P. algirus (
Contrary to DF, FID did not differ significantly between males that escaped noisily and those that escaped silently. A computer-based experiment showed that long FID was necessary for the antipredator benefit of flash behavior (
All females of A. cinerea examined in this study escaped silently. In a previous study by
This study identified factors related to the noisy escape of male A. cinerea. High ambient temperature, long DF, and intact hind legs were found to be important for the production of sound by males during their escape. These conditions indicate that the ability to escape is higher in males that produce sound than in males that do not produce sounds. Hence, the sound may act as an antipredator signal, indicating this escape ability and deterring predators from approaching the prey (
We wish to thank the members of the Laboratory of Ecological Science at Kyushu University for their valuable comments. We also thank Kyushu University for allowing us to perform the field experiment in its conservation area.